Page 42 - gt1510
P. 42
Introductory geology: Is there a common language?
GSA TODAY | OCTOBER 2015 Karen M. Kortz, Community College of Rhode Island, Physics (Bransford et al., 2000; Earth Science Literacy Initiative, 2009;
Dept., 1762 Louisquisset Pike, Lincoln, Rhode Island 02865, USA, Next Generation Science Standards, 2014).
kkortz@ccri.edu; Amber R. Caulkins, University of Rhode Island,
Dept. of Communication Studies, 101 Davis Hall, 10 Lippitt Road, Since the copious use of terminology potentially affects student
Kingston, Rhode Island 02881, USA learning, and limiting terminology requires knowing which terms
are most valued by geologists, we analyzed terms in college-level
INTRODUCTION introductory geology textbooks. In particular, we analyzed glos-
sary terms, comparing whether a common vocabulary exists
Geologic terms provide a common language for communi- between the textbooks.
cating geoscience concepts. Because introductory geoscience
students can learn only a limited number of these terms, ques- METHODS
tions arise about which terms are essential to learn and if there is
agreement between geoscientists on these terms. We tabulated glossary terms in 16 introductory physical geology
textbooks. Minor variations in terms (e.g., “P-wave” and “P wave”)
Students are frequently exposed to terms through their text- between textbooks were combined into a common term that was
books, and previous studies have examined vocabulary in texts, used during analysis. One author compiled terms, and the other
although not college-level geology textbooks. In a high school author confirmed the list.
earth science textbook, Groves (1995) found a rate of 4.45 scien-
tific terms per page. Zechmeister and Zechmeister (2000) RESULTS
compared ten college-level introductory psychology textbooks
and counted 2,505 unique terms in the glossaries, with <3% of Textbooks written by the same authors (e.g., essentials and full
terms common to all ten glossaries. versions) used a fairly consistent language, so we present the analysis
of only the full versions of ten textbooks. We note, however, that one
An extensive use of geologic terms in introductory textbooks “essentials” textbook (Marshak, 2009) had more terms in the glos-
may lead to difficulties in learning. The limited working-memory sary (1,435) than the “full” version (1,301 terms; Marshak, 2008).
capacity of most novices results in the ability to attend to only a
small amount of new information while reading, which decreases We identified 2,776 individual, unique terms in the ten
text comprehension (Sweller et al., 1998; Cain et al., 2004). If full-version textbooks, averaging 678 terms per book glossary
students are using their mental capabilities to comprehend (Table 1). To verify that the glossary terms matched the bolded
unknown terms, their cognitive systems could become over- words in textbooks, we crosschecked 10% of the glossary words
loaded, and deep cognitive processing may not happen. Students and bolded words in a subset of three textbooks and found that
may use geologic terms without fully understanding their under- 96.8% of bolded words (n = 210) were in the glossary, and 93.6%
lying concepts (Libarkin and Kurdziel, 2006; Kortz and Murray, of glossary terms (n = 203) were bolded. Italicized words
2009; Clark et al., 2011). In addition, students have less facility increased the total number of words emphasized in the text by
than experts in extracting the relevant information and seeing the 1.5 times, although they were not included in our analysis
big picture (e.g., Caillies et al., 2002; Patrick et al., 2005). because they were predominantly not in the glossary.
Therefore, students may focus on small details, such as geologic
terms, instead of using those terms to construct a holistic concep- There was minimal overlap in glossary terms between the text-
tual understanding. Students may then have an illusion of deep books. Only 44 terms (1.6% of the unique terms) were common
understanding because they can recognize vocabulary words to all ten textbooks. Examples of these 44 terms are abrasion,
(Graesser and Forsyth, 2013). barrier island, epicenter, igneous rock, joint, mantle, plate
tectonics, and volcano. Only 16.4% of terms are in five or more
Extensively incorporating terms may lead to unintended conse- textbooks, and over half of terms (55.3%) were unique to indi-
quences. For example, an emphasis on learning terms may vidual textbooks. Examples of the 39.5% of terms unique to
contribute to the misconception that science is a finished body of Marshak (2008) include dormant volcano, olistotrome, sabkah,
knowledge requiring abundant memorization (Groves, 1995). In snotite, and topsoil, whereas examples of the 8.5% of terms
addition, introducing large numbers of terms may lead to the unique to Murck et al. (2010) include fractionation, kingdom, and
emphasis on a breadth instead of a depth of knowledge, contrary seismic discontinuity. Unique terms may be used in other text-
to what has been recommended by education reformers books, but if they were not in the glossary, they were not included
in this study.
GSA Today, v. 25, no. 10, doi: 10.1130/GSATG236GW.1.
42