Page 42 - gt1510
P. 42

Introductory geology: Is there a common language?

GSA TODAY | OCTOBER 2015  Karen M. Kortz, Community College of Rhode Island, Physics            (Bransford et al., 2000; Earth Science Literacy Initiative, 2009;
                          Dept., 1762 Louisquisset Pike, Lincoln, Rhode Island 02865, USA,      Next Generation Science Standards, 2014).
                          kkortz@ccri.edu; Amber R. Caulkins, University of Rhode Island,
                          Dept. of Communication Studies, 101 Davis Hall, 10 Lippitt Road,        Since the copious use of terminology potentially affects student
                          Kingston, Rhode Island 02881, USA                                     learning, and limiting terminology requires knowing which terms
                                                                                                are most valued by geologists, we analyzed terms in college-level
                          INTRODUCTION                                                          introductory geology textbooks. In particular, we analyzed glos-
                                                                                                sary terms, comparing whether a common vocabulary exists
                            Geologic terms provide a common language for communi-               between the textbooks.
                          cating geoscience concepts. Because introductory geoscience
                          students can learn only a limited number of these terms, ques-        METHODS
                          tions arise about which terms are essential to learn and if there is
                          agreement between geoscientists on these terms.                         We tabulated glossary terms in 16 introductory physical geology
                                                                                                textbooks. Minor variations in terms (e.g., “P-wave” and “P wave”)
                            Students are frequently exposed to terms through their text-        between textbooks were combined into a common term that was
                          books, and previous studies have examined vocabulary in texts,        used during analysis. One author compiled terms, and the other
                          although not college-level geology textbooks. In a high school        author confirmed the list.
                          earth science textbook, Groves (1995) found a rate of 4.45 scien-
                          tific terms per page. Zechmeister and Zechmeister (2000)              RESULTS
                          compared ten college-level introductory psychology textbooks
                          and counted 2,505 unique terms in the glossaries, with <3% of           Textbooks written by the same authors (e.g., essentials and full
                          terms common to all ten glossaries.                                   versions) used a fairly consistent language, so we present the analysis
                                                                                                of only the full versions of ten textbooks. We note, however, that one
                            An extensive use of geologic terms in introductory textbooks        “essentials” textbook (Marshak, 2009) had more terms in the glos-
                          may lead to difficulties in learning. The limited working-memory      sary (1,435) than the “full” version (1,301 terms; Marshak, 2008).
                          capacity of most novices results in the ability to attend to only a
                          small amount of new information while reading, which decreases          We identified 2,776 individual, unique terms in the ten
                          text comprehension (Sweller et al., 1998; Cain et al., 2004). If      full-version textbooks, averaging 678 terms per book glossary
                          students are using their mental capabilities to comprehend            (Table 1). To verify that the glossary terms matched the bolded
                          unknown terms, their cognitive systems could become over-             words in textbooks, we crosschecked 10% of the glossary words
                          loaded, and deep cognitive processing may not happen. Students        and bolded words in a subset of three textbooks and found that
                          may use geologic terms without fully understanding their under-       96.8% of bolded words (n = 210) were in the glossary, and 93.6%
                          lying concepts (Libarkin and Kurdziel, 2006; Kortz and Murray,        of glossary terms (n = 203) were bolded. Italicized words
                          2009; Clark et al., 2011). In addition, students have less facility   increased the total number of words emphasized in the text by
                          than experts in extracting the relevant information and seeing the    1.5 times, although they were not included in our analysis
                          big picture (e.g., Caillies et al., 2002; Patrick et al., 2005).      because they were predominantly not in the glossary.
                          Therefore, students may focus on small details, such as geologic
                          terms, instead of using those terms to construct a holistic concep-     There was minimal overlap in glossary terms between the text-
                          tual understanding. Students may then have an illusion of deep        books. Only 44 terms (1.6% of the unique terms) were common
                          understanding because they can recognize vocabulary words             to all ten textbooks. Examples of these 44 terms are abrasion,
                          (Graesser and Forsyth, 2013).                                         barrier island, epicenter, igneous rock, joint, mantle, plate
                                                                                                tectonics, and volcano. Only 16.4% of terms are in five or more
                            Extensively incorporating terms may lead to unintended conse-       textbooks, and over half of terms (55.3%) were unique to indi-
                          quences. For example, an emphasis on learning terms may               vidual textbooks. Examples of the 39.5% of terms unique to
                          contribute to the misconception that science is a finished body of    Marshak (2008) include dormant volcano, olistotrome, sabkah,
                          knowledge requiring abundant memorization (Groves, 1995). In          snotite, and topsoil, whereas examples of the 8.5% of terms
                          addition, introducing large numbers of terms may lead to the          unique to Murck et al. (2010) include fractionation, kingdom, and
                          emphasis on a breadth instead of a depth of knowledge, contrary       seismic discontinuity. Unique terms may be used in other text-
                          to what has been recommended by education reformers                   books, but if they were not in the glossary, they were not included
                                                                                                in this study.

      GSA Today, v. 25, no. 10, doi: 10.1130/GSATG236GW.1.

42
   37   38   39   40   41   42   43   44   45   46   47